Manuscript Click here to view linked References Comparing the benefits of Caffeine, Naps and Placebo on Verbal, Motor and Perceptual Memory
Sara C. Mednick Ph.D.1, Denise J. Cai M.A.3, Jennifer Kanady B.S.1, Sean P.A.
1. University of California, San Diego, Department of Psychiatry and
Veterans Affairs San Diego Healthcare System, Research Service
2. University of California, San Diego, Department of Psychiatry and
Veterans Affairs San Diego Healthcare System, Psychology Service
3. University of California, San Diego, Department of Psychology
University of California, San DiegoLaboratory of Sleep and Behavioral NeuroscienceDepartment of Psychiatry, 9116a9500 Gilman driveLa Jolla, CA 92093office: 858-552-8585 ext-42895fax: 858-458-4201
Caffeine, the world’s most common psychoactive substance, is used by
approximately 90% of North Americans everyday. Little is known, however, about
its benefits for memory. Napping has been shown to increase alertness and
promote learning on some memory tasks. We directly compared caffeine
(200mg) with napping (60-90 minutes) and placebo on three distinct memory
processes: declarative verbal memory, procedural motor skills, and perceptual
learning. In the verbal task, recall and recognition for unassociated words were
tested after a 7hr retention period (with a between-session nap or drug
intervention). A second, different, word list was administered post-intervention
and memory was tested after a 20min retention period. The non-declarative tasks
(finger tapping task and texture discrimination task) were trained before the
intervention and then retested afterwards. Naps enhanced recall of words after a
7hr and 20min retention interval relative to both caffeine and placebo. Caffeine
significantly impaired motor learning compared to placebo and naps. Napping
produced robust perceptual learning compared with placebo; however, naps and
caffeine were not significantly different. These findings provide evidence of the
limited benefits of caffeine for memory improvement compared with napping. We
hypothesize that impairment from caffeine may be restricted to tasks that contain
explicit information; whereas strictly implicit learning is less compromised.
Caffeine, the world’s most widely consumed stimulant (Nawrot et al.
2003), is an active ingredient in coffee, tea, chocolate, sodas, and energy drinks
(the fastest growing sector of the American beverage industry)(Lovett 2005).
Modern times have led to an increase in daily, often multiple doses of caffeine, a
rise in the coffee business, and the addition of caffeine to common beverages
such as soda, bottled water, and even chewing gum. Based on the available
product usage and food consumption data, Barone and Roberts (Barone J 1996)
estimated the mean daily intake was 4 mg/kg body weight (approximately 280mg
for a 155 pound person; 16 ounces of Starbucks coffee contains 372 mg). For
the 90th percentile of caffeine users, intakes approximated 5-7 mg/kg body weight
This increasingly common use of caffeine in our society coincides with an
increasingly common trend of individuals obtaining insufficient sleep on a regular
basis. While it is difficult to ascertain the exact number of individuals who use
caffeine as a substitute for sleep in society, the 2005-2007 National Sleep
Foundation’s annual Sleep in America polls strongly suggest that Americans
regularly consume caffeine as a substitute for sleep and/or as a result of
insufficient sleep (Foundation 2005; Foundation 2006; Foundation 2006). These
polls report consistent associations between low quantity or quality of sleep,
decreased daytime functioning, and increased daytime caffeine consumption.
A few studies have examined the benefits of daytime caffeine
consumption in non-experimentally sleep-deprived individuals (Dimpfel et al.
1993; Spriet 1995; Lieberman et al. 2002). One study found 200 and 300 mg of
caffeine benefited visual vigilance, choice reaction time, repeated acquisition,
and self-reported fatigue and sleepiness, but did not improve marksmanship, a
task that requires fine motor coordination and steadiness (Lieberman et al.
2002). Dimpfel et al. measured the effects of placebo, 200 and 400 mg of
caffeine on human electroencephalogram (EEG) patterns at rest and during
mental concentration tests. In addition to the finding that the effects of caffeine
can be quantified with EEG spectral densities, they also found that subjects
achieved the best results on concentration tests when given 200 mg of caffeine.
In fact, subjects given 400mg tested below subjects in the placebo condition.
Other studies have found similar improvements on cognitive tasks with as little as
70 mg of caffeine administration compared to placebo (Rogers et al. 1995). While
these studies show caffeine can enhance wakefulness and performance on
attention and concentration tasks, little is known about the effects of caffeine on
learning and memory (Spriet 1995; Nawrot et al. 2003).
Naps, in contrast to caffeine, have been shown to enhance not only
alertness and attention, but also some forms of memory consolidation. In
particular, naps (daytime sleep between 5-90minutes) appear to improve
performance on non-medio-temporal lobe dependent, procedural skills (Mednick
et al. 2002; Mednick et al. 2003; Korman et al. 2007; Nishida et al. 2007).
Mednick and colleagues reported that a mid-day nap can also reverse perceptual
deterioration that builds with repeated within-day testing (Mednick et al. 2002).
They further showed that naps with SWS and REM produced improvements in
performance equivalent to that of a full night of sleep, whereas naps with only
SWS restored deteriorated performance to baseline levels (Mednick et al. 2003).
Walker and colleagues have demonstrated that naps improve procedural motor
skill learning to the same degree as a full night of sleep, and that improvement on
this task was correlated with Stage 2 and sleep spindle activity (Walker et al.
2004; Nishida et al. 2007). Tucker compared naps with non-REM sleep to a no-
nap condition on a procedural memory task and a declarative, verbal-paired-
associates task. They found that the non-REM naps produced improved
performance in the declarative, but not the procedural task (Tucker et al. 2006).
This is evidence that non-REM in naps can produce similar declarative memory
improvements as nocturnal non-REM sleep (Plihal et al. 1999).
Prior studies of performance during nightshift work have directly compared
caffeine and napping in on a variety of tasks (Schweitzer et al. 2006; Sagaspe et
al. 2007). For example, recently, Sagaspe and colleagues compared the effects
of a single 200mg dose of caffeine to a 30min nap and placebo on nocturnal
driving in young and middle-aged participants. They found that both interventions
significantly improved performance in both age groups, although napping was
even more effective in younger compared to older participants. There are no
studies, however, directly comparing the effects of caffeine and naps during the
day in normally-rested individuals, and few that have compared caffeine and
sleep at any time for cognitive processes beyond attention, vigilance, or driving.
Here, we compared the effects of caffeine, a daytime nap, or placebo on three
distinct memory processes: declarative verbal memory, procedural motor skills,
and perceptual learning. For verbal memory, we tested recall and recognition in
two different phases: 7hr retention with a between-session intervention (caffeine,
placebo or nap), and 20min retention for a different list of words post-
intervention. The non-declarative tasks (finger tapping task and texture
discrimination task) were trained before the intervention and then retested
61 adults between ages 18-39 with no personal history of neurological,
psychological or other chronic illness gave informed consent to participate in the
experiment. Subjects were low to moderate caffeine drinkers (no more that two
cups of coffee per day). Since restricted nighttime sleep can have a deleterious
effect on performance (Van Dongen et al. 2003), we required that subjects
maintain a sleep schedule for one week prior to the study. For seven nights prior
to the study, subjects were instructed to go to bed no later than midnight and to
get up no later than 8am. They were asked to spend at least eight hours in bed
each night. Subjects filled out sleep diaries and wore actigraphs as subjective
and objective measures of sleep-wake activity. Subjects were restricted from
consuming caffeine and alcohol 24 hours prior to and during the experimental
An uneven number of subjects were run in all three tasks due to technical
error, subjects misunderstanding the task which led to unusable data, and adding
the verbal task midway through the study. For the Verbal task, 11 placebo, 12
nappers and 12 caffeine subjects were run. For the Motor task, 18 placebo, 13
nappers and 18 caffeine subjects were run. For the Perceptual task, 19 placebo,
18 nappers and 18 caffeine subjects were run.
Figure 1 shows study timeline (an example task order scenario). Task
order was counterbalanced across subjects. Subjects were in the lab under
supervision during the entire experimental day. Subjects’ knowledge of testing
procedure was limited to being told that they would be tested in the morning and
afternoon on the all three tasks. At 0930, subjects were administered the initial
verbal task and were trained on the finger tapping task (FTT) and texture
discrimination task (TDT) (Session One). Lunch was served at noon. At 1300,
subjects were randomly assigned to a nap or a drug group. Subjects either took a
polysomnographically-recorded (PSG) nap (90-minutes of sleep maximum or up
to two hours in bed) or listened to a book on tape with PSG monitoring. A
summary of Nap PSG can be found in Table 2. At 1500, subjects in the drug
groups were given an unmarked pill (200mg caffeine or placebo). Sixty minutes
later (Session 2), subjects were tested on all three tasks, as described below.
In addition, subjective sleepiness was measured before and after each
test session with the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS). The KSS assesses
subjects’ momentary state of alertness/sleepiness on a 1-9 scale (“extremely
alert” to “extremely sleepy”). Before the first test session subjects also completed
the Epworth Sleepiness Scale. The Epworth assesses trait daytime sleepiness
with eight questions, each scored with a degree of severity ranging from 0 to 3. A
score less than 10 is considered normal. Table 1 shows the demographic
information, Epworth score, and actigraphy data from the week prior to
experimental day, including Total Sleep Time (TST), Bedtime and Waketime.
We examined recall and recognition memory in two different phases of
verbal memory: 7hr retention with a between-session intervention (caffeine,
placebo or nap), and 20min retention for a different list of words post-
intervention. During Session One, subjects were trained and tested on Word List
1. During training, the experimenter read aloud 24 unrelated words in three
consecutive trials. Immediately after each trial, subjects were asked to recall the
words. After a period of 20 minutes (during which non-verbal tasks were
completed), subjects were given tests of free recall and recognition for Word List
1. No feedback on performance was given. In the recognition test, subjects were
read aloud a list of 48 words (half the words were Word List 1 and half were
lures) and determined which were on Word List 1.
At the start of the second test session, tests of recall and recognition were
given for Word List 1 in order to test for 7hr retention. Afterwards, the entire
verbal memory task was repeated with Word List 2 to test for 20min retention in
recall and recognition memory. For each test session, free recall was measured
as the number of words correctly recalled unprompted. Recognition memory
performance was measured with d’ (index of discriminability between target and
lure words). We used two of the word lists here that were previously developed
for other studies of verbal learning in our lab (Stricker et al. 2006). Words were
chosen from those normed for recallability by Christian et al. (1978) (Christian
1978), and each list was matched for recallability, word length, concreteness,
and imagery. List order was counterbalanced across subjects.
The finger tapping task (FTT) was identical to that from Walker (Walker et
al. 2002). The task required subjects to repeatedly complete, with their left (non-
dominant) hand, the sequence 4-1-3-2-4 on a keyboard. Each block consisted of
30 seconds of key presses followed by 30 seconds of rest. The training session
consisted of 12 blocks and the test session consisted of 3 blocks. The numeric
sequence (4-1-3-2-4) was displayed at the top of the screen at all times to
exclude any working memory component to the task. Each key press produced
a white dot below, forming a row from left to right over the course of each key
press sequence. Performance was measured as the number of correct
sequences completed (score), and number of errors made (accuracy)
Participants performed a texture discrimination task similar to that
developed by Karni and Sagi (Karni et al. 1991) and identical to that utilized in
our previous studies (Mednick et al. 2002; Mednick et al. 2003; Mednick et al.
2005). Participants were asked to discriminate two targets per trial: a central
letter (‘T’ or ‘L’), and a peripheral line array (vertical or horizontal orientation) in
the lower left quadrant at 2.5-5.9 deg eccentricity from the center of the screen.
The peripheral array consisted of three diagonal bars that were either positioned
in a horizontal array or a vertical array against a background of horizontally
oriented bars, which created a texture difference between the target and
An experimental trial consisted of the following sequence: central fixation
cross, target screen for 32 ms, blank screen for a duration between 0 and 600
ms (the inter-stimulus-interval, or ISI), mask for 16 ms followed by the response
time interval before the next trial. Subjects reported both the letter at central
fixation (T or L) and the orientation of the peripheral, three-element array
(horizontal or vertical) by making two key presses. The central task controlled for
Each block consisted of 50 trials, each with the same ISI, and lasting
approximately 2 minutes. A threshold was determined from the performance
across 20 blocks, with a progressively shorter ISI, starting with 600 msec and
ending with 0 msec. The specific sequence of ISIs across an entire session was
[600, 500, 400, 350, 300, 250, 200, 175, 150, 125, 100, 80, 60, 40, 20, 0]. A
psychometric function of percent correct for each block was fit with a Weibull
function to determine the ISI at which performance yielded 80% accuracy.
Participants controlled the onset of each block and were instructed to take
as many breaks as they needed between blocks. Once a block began, a new trial
initiated every 2 seconds, regardless of whether or not the subject made a
response. Training, which occurred at the beginning of the 9AM test session,
consisted of 15 trials of an easy version of the task (ISI of 1000 – 1500 msec),
and 50 trials of the easiest block of the actual task (ISI of 600 msec). This
training ensured that participants understood the task and were discriminating
the peripheral target between 90% and 100% correct on the easiest version of
Our main outcome of interest involved the recall and recognition memory
scores for the 7hr retention interval, since that interval included the different
interventions. To examine that, we utilized a one-way analysis of variance
(ANOVA) using three levels of the variable Group (Caffeine, Nap, Placebo),
separately for recall and recognition. One concern with this approach, though,
would be whether the three groups showed equal performance at baseline (i.e.,
20min memory for Word List 1). Thus, we first evaluated that question with a
similar 1-way ANOVA. If that analysis showed a significant main effect of group,
we planned to control for baseline performance by examining the Session x
Group interaction in a repeated measures ANOVA. However, since neither 20min
recall nor recognition showed baseline differences (see Results, below), we
utilized the 1-way ANOVAs for the 7hr retention interval to maximize power for
our main effect of interest. Finally, to examine the impact of the intervention on
the ability to encode new words, we conducted the same analysis for recall and
recognition of Word List 2 at the 20min retention interval. Significant ANOVAs
were followed-up by examining differences between groups at the specific time
Prior to conducting the response time (RT) analyses described below,
errors and a small number of extreme outlier trials (RTs of greater than 3000 ms)
were excluded (Walker et al. 2002). We examined group differences across
Session One and Two (i.e., learning) with a Repeated-Measures ANOVA, with
Group as the between-subject variable, and Session (mean performance from
last two blocks of the training vs. two blocks of test) as the within-subjects
variable. This ANOVA was conducted for both Score and Accuracy.
We examined group differences across Session One and Two with a
Repeated-Measures ANOVA, with Group as the between-subjects variable, and
session thresholds as within-subjects variables (Mednick et al. 2003).
Sleepiness was examined with a mixed-model repeated measured
ANOVA with Group as the between variable and the four administrations as the
within-factors. Also, we specifically examined the KSS rating from immediately
after the treatment in a one-way ANOVA to examine acute treatment effects of
No significant differences were found between groups in Recall of Word
List 1 at 20min (Recall means and standard deviations = 13.70(3.0), 15.25(3.33),
12.25(3.5) for placebo, nap, caffeine, respectively; F=2.36, p=.11, eta2=.12) or
Recognition of Word List 1 at 20min (Recognition means and standard deviations
= 4.5(.99), 4.9(.60), 4.5(.73), for placebo, nap, caffeine, respectively; F=.73,
p=.49, eta2=.04). Recall memory for Word List 1 after 7hr retention interval
showed significant group differences (F=5.41 p=.009, partial eta2=.25, Fig 2a).
Post-hoc tests showed: a) the Nap group performed significantly better than the
Caffeine group (p=.003); b) Nap performed marginally better than Placebo
(p=.06.); and c) there were non-significant differences between Caffeine and
Placebo (p=.22). Recognition memory for words after a 7hr retention interval also
showed significant group differences for d’ (F=4.51 p=.019, partial eta2 =.22, Fig
2b). Post-hoc tests showed: a) Nap performed significantly better than Caffeine
(p=.008); b) Nap better than Placebo (p=.03); and c) no difference between
Recall after a 20min retention interval showed significant group
differences (F=4.97 p=.01, partial eta2=.24, Figure 2c). Post-hoc tests showed: a)
Nap performed significantly better than Caffeine (p=.004); b) no difference
between Nap and Placebo (p=.21); and c) Caffeine performed marginally worse
than Placebo (p=.08). For recognition memory after a 20min retention interval, no
Group differences were found for d’ (F=.57 p=.57, partial eta2 =.03, Fig 2d). Data
from the Verbal Task is shown in Figure 2.
A Repeated Measures ANOVA on Accuracy showed no significant
interaction between group and accuracy (F=1.87, p=.16, partial eta2=.07).
Accuracy was consistently high for all groups. Mean accuracy for the last two
blocks of training was .97, .97, and .98 for the placebo, nap and caffeine groups,
respectively. For the two blocks of the test session, these values were .98, .98,
A Repeated Measures ANOVA on Score was statistically significant,
(F=6.14, p=.004, partial eta2=.21). Post-hoc one-sample t-tests on the differences
scores (last two blocks of train session minus first two blocks of test session)
indicated the caffeine group showed significantly impaired learning (i.e., smaller
increase in the number of sequences completed at Session 2) compared with
placebo (p = .003), and nappers (p=.03). No difference was found in between
nap and placebo (p=.38). Indeed the caffeine group did not show improvement
across sessions (p=.43), whereas nappers (p=.000) and placebo (p=.000) groups
showed significantly higher scores. Difference scores are shown in Figure 3.
`Performance improvement across the three groups was examined with a
Repeated-Measures ANOVA. There was a marginally significant difference
across three the groups (F=2.44, p=.09, eta2=.09). Post-hoc one-sample t-tests
on the difference scores indicate that nappers showed the typical improvement
on the TDT compared with placebo (p=.02). However, the caffeine group fell in-
between naps and placebo and was not significantly different from either naps
(p=.29) or placebo (p=.26). Difference scores are shown in Figure 4.
There was a marginally significant group effect on subjective sleepiness
ratings across all four administrations (F=2.77, p=.07, eta2=.09). Compared to
naps and placebo, caffeine subjects reported being more alert immediately prior
to the testing session following the intervention. Sleepiness rating show
significant group differences (F=3.90, p=.03, eta2=.20, Figure 5) during this third
In this study, we find that a moderate dose of caffeine impaired motor
sequence learning and declarative verbal memory compared to placebo and
daytime sleep. These decreases were found despite the fact that caffeine
increased subjective alertness, suggesting that the caffeine dose was sufficiently
high to have some psychoactive effect. An afternoon nap, on the other hand,
improved free recall memory relative to the caffeine group after both a 20min and
a 7hr retention interval and produced greater learning on a motor sequence task
than caffeine. Although napping produced improvements in the perceptual and
motor tasks similar to that previously reported, we also found large amounts of
learning in the placebo condition. In the perceptual task, the placebo group
showed significantly better performance than previous studies have reported in
the non-nap control groups (Mednick et al. 2003). Specifically non-nap controls
typically show performance deterioration with repeated testing, whereas the
placebo controls in the present study showed no deterioration. Furthermore, the
level of improvements on the motor task in the placebo group is larger than
control group performance in prior studies for both nocturnal sleep (Walker et al.
2002) and naps (Nishida et al. 2007). We hypothesize, at least for the motor and
perceptual tasks, that the placebo condition produced a true “placebo effect” on
Data from the Verbal Memory task suggest a sleep-dependent
consolidation process occurs during the nap that allows for better recall and a
finer discrimination between targets and distracters than can be achieved when
sleep does not occur between study and test periods or by caffeine. It should be
noted the present findings suggest a possible role for sleep during naps in
declarative memory consolidation of unassociated, rather than associated,
words. Recent research has shown that associative and non-associative
declarative memory may rely on separate brain regions (Vargha-Khadem et al.
1997; Mayes et al. 2002; Turriziani et al. 2004). Specifically, these studies assign
item memory formation (non-associative) to the parahippocampal gyrus
(particularly rhinal cortices: anterior parahippocampal gyrus and
parahippocampal cortex) and associative memory formation to the hippocampus.
The majority of prior studies examining hippocampal-related, sleep-dependent
memory have investigated memory for associated word-pairs (Gais et al. 2002;
Drosopoulos et al. 2005; Backhaus et al. 2007). Instead, the present study
examined item memory consolidation, which relies on parahippocampal and
rhinal cortices. These findings expand the growing literature on the relationship
between memory and sleep to suggest that sleep may benefit declarative
memory consolidation not limited to processes subserved by the hippocampus
itself. In addition to enhancing memory consolidation for previously studied
words, naps improved the ability to learn a new list of words post-intervention
In this study, caffeine decreased subjective sleepiness. This enhanced
alertness, however, did not seem to transfer to motor learning and verbal
memory. Although 200mg of caffeine is considered a moderate dose, other
studies have also found similar doses impaired motor skill. In a complex test of
hand-eye coordination in which subjects had to insert a stylus successively into
three holes, 60 or 120mg of caffeine decreased, while 180 or 240mg of caffeine
increased the time-to-task-completion (Hollingworth 1912). In another study,
reading time of text increased with 2 or 4mg of caffeine per kilogram of body
weight (MacPherson et al. 1996). Other studies have similarly shown that
caffeine is unable to reverse the effects of sleep-deprivation on areas of higher
level cognition, for example disadvantageous, high-risk decision making (Killgore
WD 2007). A study of Navy Seals during the highly stressful training period (i.e.
Hell Week) demonstrated that caffeine improved vigilance and speeded-reaction
time (Lieberman et al. 2002). However, it was less effective for more complex
cognitive tasks, such as working memory, marksmanship accuracy and time to
sight the target. Other studies have noted a similar lack of efficacy for caffeine in
higher cognitive tasks (Battig K 1984; Amendola CA 1998). This study extends
these previous findings by being the first study, of which we are aware, to show
reduced motor memory consolidation with caffeine.
Although caffeine is clearly effective in increasing arousal, the studies
cited above indicate that the perceived cognitive benefit of caffeine may not
universally translate to objective performance. High consumers of caffeine
demonstrate faster simple and choice reaction times and report positive
subjective effects in response to caffeine administration. Moderate to low users,
on the other hand, do not demonstrate these enhancements (Attwood et al.
2007). Performance improvements from caffeine in some studies may thus
represent a relief from withdrawal symptoms in high users. Consistent with this
withdrawal hypothesis, we show no benefit to memory performance with caffeine,
even in moderate consumers of caffeine (100-200mg per day). One could also
argue based on these data that this relatively low dose of caffeine actually
slightly impairs the ability to learn new information (Fig 2c, Fig 3). Such an
impairment of performance, if replicated, runs counter to the general society
assumption that caffeine typically benefits cognitive performance (in this case,
One limitation of this study is that only one dose of caffeine was
administered. Thus, the findings should not be generalized beyond this single
dose of caffeine, roughly equivalent to two - three cups of coffee. Future studies
using multiple doses may show a dose-response effect on motor and verbal
memory. It is possible that a group receiving either a higher or lower (than 200
mg) dose of caffeine would exhibit more optimal arousal states and relatively
improved performance relative to the placebo or nap group. Similarly, future
studies may wish to use multiple doses of sleep (i.e., different lengths of sleep
opportunity) to also test whether a dose response relationship exists with
napping for verbal memory as reported for perceptual learning (Mednick et al.
One possible explanation for the motor decrements reported in the
present study is that caffeine impairs local motor movements. Typically, it is
thought that at least 5mg/kg is needed to produce hand tremors. But a few
studies have found even lower doses can induce tremors (Paroli 1972; Jacobson
BH 1987). The moderate dose of 200mg may also have caused a global over
stimulation to the nervous system, even without overt hand tremors, which
impaired performance. Although subjects reported typically consuming 100-
200mg of caffeine a day, this is likely absorbed through a caffeinated beverage.
Oral administration of the pill may have increased arousal more suddenly than
sipping a cup of coffee. This heightened increase in arousal may have impaired
learning, as the Yerkes-Dodson law states that performance is poor at high and
It is possible that listening to a book on tape during the time interval
reserved for sleep in the napping group may have caused interference in the
caffeine and placebo groups for the verbal memory task. However, the fact that
the book on tape started approximately 2 hours after the morning test session
was completed may have reduced this possibility. If such interference did occur,
the caffeine group appeared to suffer more from interference than the placebo
group, since the caffeine group performed marginally worse than the placebo
group for the 20min Recall of Word List 2. Finally, the choice of a language-
based activity for this control period has the advantage of preventing (or at least
reducing) rehearsal of Word List 1 in the drug groups, which would have
potentially increased memory consolidation in those groups independent of the
Overall, a daytime nap generally improved performance across three
different learning paradigms, while caffeine impaired (or at least did not benefit)
performance. We hypothesize that the pattern of results demonstrated by the
caffeine group may be explained by the relative level of explicit information in
each memory task. The three tasks, perceptual learning, procedural motor skill,
and verbal memory, each have varying levels of explicit information involved in
learning. The perceptual learning task involves the least amount of explicit
material, as demonstrated by the high degree of specificity shown in performance
profiles (Mednick et al. 2005) and no conscious access to learning or
deterioration (Mednick et al. 2002). The motor task, although procedural, shows
a strong explicit component, in that explicit sequence knowledge has been
shown to modify off-line consolidation (Robertson et al. 2004). Also, subjects
report consciously practicing the specific sequence between training and test
(Rickard 2007). The verbal task is by nature an explicit task in which subjects
must consciously hold on to individual test words for later recall.
Explicitness in memory tasks has been shown to be related to the degree
that the task engages the hippocampus (Greene 2007). Sleep-dependent
memory improvement in hippocampal-related tasks appears to be reliant on
SWS (Gais et al. 2000). In particular, Gais and Born have demonstrated that low
acetylcholine during SWS is important for explicit verbal memory (Gais et al.
2004), but not implicit memory. Acetylcholine naturally decreases during sleep,
whereas caffeine has been shown to increase hippocampal acetylcholine via
antagonism of local adenosine A1 receptors (Carter et al. 1995). This increase in
hippocampal acetylcholine by caffeine may block the consolidation process by
blocking replay of new memories. Consistent with this conceptualization, we
found that the greater the explicit component of each task, the worse the caffeine
Recent attention to the importance of overnight sleep for a variety of
health and cognitive domains has demonstrated that no complete
pharmacological alternative to a good night’s rest has been discovered. The
present findings suggest that caffeine, the most common pharmacological
intervention for sleepiness, may not be an adequate substitute for the memory
enhancements of daytime sleep, either.
We would like to acknowledge Kathy Resovsky, Ryan Wong, Arlene Schlosser for help conducting the study; Robert Stickgold and John Wixted for their thoughtful comments. Research was supported by DARPA award # N0014-06-1-0660 , the UCSD GCRC M01 RR00827, K01 MH080992-01, R01-AG024506
Figure 1: Experimental timeline. All subjects tested on Word List 1 in the morning. At 1pm, nappers slept with PSG monitoring. At 3pm non-nappers received an unmarked pill (200mg of caffeine or placebo). All subjects retested on Word List 1 after 7hr retention interval. All subjects were then trained and tested on Word List 2 with a 20min retention interval.
Figure 2. Declarative Verbal Memory Task. Verbal memory performance in Placebo (black bar), Nap (striped bar), and Caffeine groups (grey bar) (means and standard errors), with p-values of significant group differences. 7hr retention of Morning Words in Recall (2a) and Recognition in d’ (2b), 20min retention of Evening Words in Recall (2c) and Recognition in d’ (2d).
Figure 3: Motor Skill Learning. Differences Scores on Finger Tapping task represent increase in number of correct sequences completed in session two compared with session one in placebo (black bar), Nap (striped bar) and caffeine (grey bar) groups.
Figure 4. Perceptual Learning Task. Threshold Difference Score for Texture Discrimination task represents change in threshold from session one to session two in placebo (black bar), Nap (striped bar) and caffeine (grey bar) groups.
Figure 4. Subjective sleepiness ratings across the day for each group (averages and standard errors). Caffeine group showed a short period of increased alertness immediately after a dose of caffeine (*).
WORD LIST 1 TESTWORD LIST 2 TRAINFTTWORD LIST 2 TESTTDT
2a. Recall 7hr Delay 2b. Recognition 7hr Delay lle a c e R 12 2c. Recall 20min Delay 2d. Recognition 20min Delay
Figure 2. Verbal memory performance in Placebo (black bar), Nap (striped
bar), and Caffeine groups (grey bar) (means and standard errors), with p-
values of significant group differences. 7hr retention of Morning Words in
Recall (2a) and Recognition in d’ (2b), 20min retention of Evening Words in
Recall (2c) and Recognition in d’ (2d). iffe 3 r D to o M 2
Figure 3: Motor Skill Learning. Differences Scores on Finger Tapping task represent increase in number of correct sequences completed in session two compared with session one in placebo (black bar), Nap (striped bar) and caffeine (grey bar) groups. c S e c n re 25 iffe D ld 15
Figure 4. Texture Discrimination Task across Groups. Threshold Difference
Score represents change in threshold from session one to session two.
Threshold Difference Scores above zero indicate learning. re o c S s s e in p e le S a k s lin ro a K
Figure 5. Subjective sleepiness ratings across the day for each group (averages and standard errors) Although no significant Group differences were found across session, the Placebo group reported increased sleepiness across the day; the Caffeine group showed a short period of decreased sleepiness immediately after a dose of caffeine (*); the Nappers remained at the same level of sleepiness across all time points.
Table 1 Demographics and Actigraphy (Mean and Standard Deviation)
Table 2 Polysomography of Naps (Mean and Standard Deviation)
Amendola CA, G. J., Lieberman HR (1998). "Caffeine’s effects on performance
and mood are independent of age and gender." Nutr Neurosci 1: 269-280.
Attwood, A. S., S. Higgs, et al. (2007). "Differential responsiveness to caffeine
and perceived effects of caffeine in moderate and high regular caffeine consumers." Psychopharmacology (Berl) 190(4): 469-77.
Backhaus, J., J. Born, et al. (2007). "Midlife decline in declarative memory
consolidation is correlated with a decline in slow wave sleep." Learn Mem 14(5): 336-41.
Barone J, r. H. (1996). "Caffeine Consumption." Food Chem Toxicol 34: 119-29. Battig K, B. R., Martin JR, Feierabend JM (1984). "The effects of caffeine on
physiological functions and mental performance." Experientia 40: 1218– 1223.
Carter, A. J., W. T. O'Connor, et al. (1995). "Caffeine enhances acetylcholine
release in the hippocampus in vivo by a selective interaction with adenosine A1 receptors." J Pharmacol Exp Ther 273(2): 637-42.
Christian, J., Bickley, W., Tarka, M., Clayton, K. (1978). "Measures of free recall
nouns: Correlations with imagery, concreteness,
meaningfulness, and frequency." Memory and Cognition 6: 379-390.
Dimpfel, W., F. Schober, et al. (1993). "The influence of caffeine on human EEG
under resting conditions and during mental loads." Clin Investig 71(3): 197-207.
Drosopoulos, S., U. Wagner, et al. (2005). "Sleep enhances explicit recollection
in recognition memory." Learn Mem 12(1): 44-51.
Foundation, N. S. (2005). Sleep in America Poll. Foundation, N. S. (2006). Sleep in America Poll.
Foundation, N. S. (2006). "Sleep in America Poll."Gais, S. and J. Born (2004). "Low acetylcholine during slow-wave sleep is critical
for declarative memory consolidation." Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101(7): 2140-4.
Gais, S., M. Molle, et al. (2002). "Learning-dependent increases in sleep spindle
density." J Neurosci 22(15): 6830-4.
Gais, S., W. Plihal, et al. (2000). "Early sleep triggers memory for early visual
discrimination skills." Nature Neuroscience 3: 1335-1339.
Greene, A. J. (2007). "Human hippocampal-dependent tasks: is awareness
necessary or sufficient?" Hippocampus 17(6): 429-33.
Hollingworth, H. L. (1912). "The influence of caffeine on mental and motor
efficiency. ." Arch. Psychol. 3: 1–166
Jacobson BH, E. B. (1987). "Effects of caffeine on simple reaction time and
movement time." Avia Space Environment Med 58: 1153-1156.
Karni, A. and D. Sagi (1991). "Where practice makes perfect in texture
discrimination: evidence for primary visual cortex plasticity." Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America 88: 4966-4970.
Killgore WD, L. E., Kamimori GH, Balkin TJ. (2007). "Caffeine effects on risky
decision making after 75 hours of sleep deprivation." Aviat Space Environ Med. 78(10): 957-62.
Korman, M., J. Doyon, et al. (2007). "Daytime sleep condenses the time course
of motor memory consolidation." Nat Neurosci 10(9): 1206-13.
Lieberman, H., W. Tharion, et al. (2002). "Effects of caffeine, sleep loss, and
stress on cognitive performance and mood during U.S. Navy SEAL training." Psychopharmacology 164(3): 250-261.
Lieberman, H. R., W. J. Tharion, et al. (2002). "Effects of caffeine, sleep loss,
and stress on cognitive performance and mood during U.S. Navy SEAL training. Sea-Air-Land." Psychopharmacology (Berl) 164(3): 250-61.
Lovett, R. (2005). "Coffee: The demon drink?" New Scientist 24: 2518-2522. MacPherson, J., S. Sternhagen, et al. (1996). "Effect of caffeine, impulsivity, and
gender on the components of text processing and recall." Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 4(4): 438-446.
Mayes, A. R., J. S. Holdstock, et al. (2002). "Relative sparing of item recognition
memory in a patient with adult-onset damage limited to the hippocampus." Hippocampus 12(3): 325-40.
Mednick, S., K. Nakayama, et al. (2003). "Sleep-dependent learning: a nap is as
good as a night." Nat Neurosci 6(7): 697-8.
Mednick, S. C., A. C. Arman, et al. (2005). "The time course and specificity of
perceptual deterioration." Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102(10): 3881-5.
Mednick, S. C., K. Nakayama, et al. (2002). "The restorative effect of naps on
perceptual deterioration." Nat Neurosci 5(7): 677-81.
Nawrot, P., S. Jordan, et al. (2003). "Effects of caffeine on human health." Food
Addit Contam 20(1): 1-30.
Nishida, M. and M. P. Walker (2007). "Daytime naps, motor memory
consolidation and regionally specific sleep spindles." PLoS ONE 2(4): e341.
Paroli, E. (1972). "Psychopharmacological aspects of coffee and of caffeine."
Minerva Med. Guil. 63: 3319–3323.
Plihal, W. and J. Born (1999). "Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on
priming and spatial memory." Psychophysiology 36: 571-82.
Rickard, T. C., Cai, D. J., (2007). Robertson, E. M., A. Pascual-Leone, et al. (2004). "Awareness modifies the skill-
learning benefits of sleep." Curr Biol 14(3): 208-12.
Rogers, P. J., N. J. Richardson, et al. (1995). "Overnight caffeine abstinence and
negative reinforcement of preference for caffeine-containing drinks." Psychopharmacology (Berl) 120(4): 457-62.
Sagaspe, P., J. Taillard, et al. (2007). "Aging and nocturnal driving: better with
coffee or a nap? A randomized study." Sleep 30(12): 1808-13.
Schweitzer, P. K., A. C. Randazzo, et al. (2006). "Laboratory and field studies of
naps and caffeine as practical countermeasures for sleep-wake problems associated with night work." Sleep 29(1): 39-50.
Spriet, L. L. (1995). "Caffeine and performance." Int J Sport Nutr 5 Suppl: S84-
Stricker, J. L., G. G. Brown, et al. (2006). "The impact of sleep deprivation and
task difficulty on networks of fMRI brain response." J Int Neuropsychol Soc 12(5): 591-7.
Tucker, M. A., Y. Hirota, et al. (2006). "A daytime nap containing solely non-REM
sleep enhances declarative but not procedural memory." Neurobiol Learn Mem.
Turriziani, P., L. Fadda, et al. (2004). "Recognition memory for single items and
for associations in amnesic patients." Neuropsychologia 42(4): 426-33.
Van Dongen, H. P., G. Maislin, et al. (2003). "The cumulative cost of additional
wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation." Sleep 26(2): 117-26.
Vargha-Khadem, F., D. G. Gadian, et al. (1997). "Differential effects of early
hippocampal pathology on episodic and semantic memory." Science 277: 376-380.
Walker, M. P., T. Brakefield, et al. (2002). "Practice with Sleep Makes Perfect:
Sleep-Dependent Motor Skill Learning." Neuron 35(1): 205-211.
Walker, M. P., T. Brakefield, et al. (2002). "Practice with sleep makes perfect:
sleep-dependent motor skill learning." Neuron 35(1): 205-11.
Walker, M. P. and R. Stickgold (2004). "Sleep-dependent learning and memory
consolidation." Neuron 44(1): 121-33.
UNION NATIONALE DE LA PETITE ET MOYENNE INDUSTRIE PREVENIR LES PME DE LA CONTREFACON ! Rapport réalisé par l’UNPMI Sous la direction de Monsieur Pierre WAINTRAUB Président de la Fédération Française de la Maroquinerie Avec la collaboration de Monsieur Dominique BROGGIO Juriste au sein de la Direction des Affaires Économiques de la CGPME Document élab
Experience the Health Benefits - Extra 20 v2 Resist Diseases1 The scientist found that the key of hundred diseases are from free radicals. In the bodily, cells are attacked by the free radical over 70,000 times everyday. Cells lose its normal function by oxidization and cause other diseases. Tongkat Ali contains anti- oxidant properties, a kind of anti-oxidant enzyme that has the abilit