Alma Mater Studiorum University of Bologna, August 22-26 2006 LANGUAGE USE IN AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY FOR MUSICAL EXPERIENCES Dr. Mark Lammers Dr. Mark Kruger ABSTRACT
opment of the self, social bonding, and motivation (Bluck,
Memories for musical experiences among adults associated
2003; Pillemer, 2003). Because music is a pervasive part of
with a symphony orchestra either as a professional per-
most individuals’ lives (Sloboda & O’Neill, 2001) and mu-
former, as an amateur performer, or as a member of an
sic has been connected to peak emotional experiences
audience were examined in order to explore the content
(Lowis, 1998; Gabrielson, 2001), it is interesting to ask if
and function of autobiographical memories. Transcripts
memory for musical experience is similar to other autobio-
for early, strong, and practice related memories were com-
graphical memories and whether it differs as a function of
puter coded for number of words and proportions of cogni-
musical expertise. Our study examines memory for musical
tion, emotion, and social relationship words. Although full-
experiences among adults associated with a symphony or-
time musicians, civic orchestra members, and audience
chestra either professionally, as an amateur member of a
members did not differ in reported ease of recall, perform-
civic orchestra, or as a member of the audience in order to
ers produced longer narratives (p < .05). Performers used
explore the content and function of autobiographical
proportionately more words associated with insight, espe-cially in their narratives about practice. Gender, but not
Research that has been done on music and autobiographical
expertise, influenced the use of emotion words (p < .05).
memory has focused on the ability of music to cue recall for events from the past and/or the impact of emotion on
Keywords
memory. For example, Schulkind, Hennis, and Rubin (1999) found that music, which was popular from an indi-
Autobiographical Memory, Musical Development, Expertise.
vidual’s adolescence and early adult years, serves as an
INTRODUCTION
effective cue to autobiographical memory. This is espe-cially true for older adults when the music is associated
Autobiographical memory is believed to aid in the devel-
with emotion. In another study, Ashley and Luce (2004)
In: M. Baroni, A. R. Addessi, R. Caterina, M. Costa (2006) Proceedings
compared the autobiographical memories which college
of the 9th International Conference on Music Perception & Cognition
students produced in response to a musical selection or an
(ICMPC9), Bologna/Italy, August 22-26 2006.2006 The Society for Music Perception & Cognition (SMPC) and European Society for the
object or memento that they associated with their high
Cognitive Sciences of Music (ESCOM). Copyright of the content of an
school years. Individuals chose their own musical selection
individual paper is held by the primary (first-named) author of that pa-
or memento. Musical selections cued autobiographical
per. All rights reserved. No paper from this proceedings may be repro-
memories with stronger emotional content, measured
duced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or me-chanical, including photocopying, recording, or by any information
physiologically (skin temperature and blood pulse ampli-
retrieval systems, without permission in writing from the paper's primary
tude) than did mementos, but mementos produced more
author. No other part of this proceedings may be reproduced or transmit-
ted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or by any information retrieval system, without
Are the autobiographical memories of musicians influenced
permission in writing from SMPC and ESCOM.
by their expertise and avocation? Kruger, Lammers, and
Ackil (2004) asked professional symphony musicians,
was 46.3. The mean age of the older audience group was
members of a civic orchestra, and members of a symphony
70.7. Eighty-three percent of the audience members re-
audience to write descriptions of three memories: an early
ported that they had studied an instrument at some time
experience, a very strong or memorable experience, and a
during their life. Twenty-two percent reported that they
meaningful experience associated with practicing an in-
currently performed in an ensemble and were excluded
strument. Each participant was also asked how old they
from further analysis. The analyses reported in this paper
were at the time of each event they recalled and a series of
compare the 20 full time professionals, 17 members of a
questions about their emotional/motivational responses,
civic orchestra, and 26 adult audience members under the
about the social context, and whether this was a memory
that they either reflected on themselves or sometimes
shared with others. Professional musicians were more
Procedure
likely to report that they had shared memories of their early
Each participant wrote short descriptions of three types of
experiences with others and that the early experience they
musical experiences- an early experience, a very strong or
recalled had motivated them to perform. Full-time mem-
memorable experience, and a meaningful experience asso-
bers were also more likely to say their memory for a strong
ciated with practicing an instrument. After writing their
experience motivated their subsequent performance. Per-
descriptions, each participant was asked how old they were
formers (professionals and civic orchestra members) as
at the time of the remembered event and a series of ques-
opposed to audience members were more likely to describe
tions about their emotional/motivational responses, about
a strong memory as one in which they played in an ensem-
the social context, and whether this was a memory that they
either reflected on themselves or sometimes shared with
Pennebaker, Mehl, and Niederhoffer (2003) argue that
others. All participants were asked to indicate the age at
analysis of word choice in spoken or written discourse can
which they first studied a musical instrument and what in-
be a reliable and valid method of measuring individual dif-
ferences, the impact of social contexts on motivation and
emotion, and the outcomes of therapeutic interventions.
Autobiographical memories for musical experiences were
Their approach is to search text for over 2300 words that
assessed using the Pennebaker, Francis, and Booth (2001)
have been associated with one of 70 linguistic categories of
Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count software. Each narra-
words. Unlike, coding schemes that require judgments
tive was coded for number of words, for the proportion of
about the meaning of a narrative, this approach relies on
positive and negative emotion words, for the proportion of
looking for pronouns, articles, prepositions, emotion words
cognition words related to causality and insight, and for the
and cognition words that suggest causality, abstraction, and
proportion of words related to social relationships.
time. Pennebaker, Francis, and Booth (2001) developed a
software system, Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count
(LIWC 2001) that can be used to analyze text.
The impact of expertise, type of event recalled, and gender
The present study reports the analysis of the autobiographi-
on total number of words, the proportion of emotion words
cal memories for early, strong, and practice related events
(positive and negative), the proportion of cognition words,
using the LIWC software system. Based on subjects self-
and the proportion of social relationship words used in
descriptions of their memories, we decided to examine dif-
each narrative was assessed using a three (Expertise: pro-
ferences in the emotions associated with individual memo-
fessional, amateur, audience) by three (Event: early, strong,
ries, the extent to which social relationships were important
practice) by two (Gender: male, female) ANOVA with one
in the experiences recalled, the extent to which memories
repeated measure (Event). Although full-time musicians,
were tied to cognitions related to cause and insight to de-
civic orchestra members, and audience members did not
termine the extent to which memories are tied to life chang-
differ in reported ease of recall, performers produced
longer narratives than members of the audience (F(2,
58)=3.96, p < .03, eta2=.12). This effect can be seen in Fig-
ure 1, which includes the older audience members. There
Participants
was also a main effect for the type of event recalled, F(2, 116)=4.22, p < .02, eta2=.07. Post-hoc tests indicate a sig-
Twenty full-time symphony musicians, eighteen adult
nificant (p<.05) differences between the number of words
members of a civic orchestra, and seventy-two adult mem-
in the strong experience event and practice related event.
bers of a concert audience served as participants in this study. The audience was divided into two age groups, those
under the median audience age of 62 and those over the median audience age. The first audience group had an av-erage age of 49.8. The average age of the professional musicians was 43.6 and the average age of the amateurs
Neither expertise nor type of event recalled produced sig-nificant differences in the proportion of emotion words, the
DISCUSSION
proportion of positive emotion words, or the proportion of
These results extend our earlier findings that professional
negative emotion words. A main effect for gender was
musicians are more likely to report events they found moti-
found in the proportion of negative emotion words used
vating, events they continued to share with others, and
(F(1,58)=4.14, p < .05, eta2=.07). The mean proportion of
events involving ensemble performance. Expertise in-
negative emotion words was 1.23 (standard error = .17) for
creases use of words associated with cognitive mecha-
females and .66 (standard error= .22) for males.
nisms. This may reflect an abstraction of experience. Ob-served gender differences are consistent with other research
A main effect for type of event recalled on the number of
on autobiographical memories (Bauer, Stennes, and Haight,
social relationship words used (F(2,116)=10.88, p<.001,
eta2=.16). The means and standard errors for the early, strong, and practice related events were 7.23(.62),
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
4.88(.57), and 3.39(.67) respectively. Neither expertise nor
Special thanks to Diane Pope, Dr. Jennifer Ackil, and two
gender produced significant differences in the proportion
of our students at Gustavus Adolphus College, Eric
of words associated with social relationships.
Wilberg and Keri Asp, for their help with this project.
Expertise produced differences (p < .05) in the proportion of
REFERENCES
(F(2,58)=3.19, p<05, eta2=.10). A similar effect was found for words associated with insight (F(2,58)=4.63, p<02,
Ashley, R. & Luce, K. (2004) Music, autobiographical
eta2=.14). Performers used proportionately more words
memory, and emotion. In S.D. Libscomb, R. Ashley, R.O.
associated with insight, especially in their narratives about
Gjerdingen, & P. Webster (Eds.) Proceedings of the 8th
practice. The means are confidence intervals are shown in
International Conference on Music Perception and Cogni-tion, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL.
Bauer, P.J. (2003). Representation of inner self in autobi-ography: Women’s and men’s use of internal state lan-guage in personal narratives. Memory, 11(1), 27.42.
Bluck, S. (2003). Autobiographical memory: Exploring its functions in everyday life. Memory, 11(2), 113-123.
Gabrielson, A. (2001). Emotions in strong experiences with music. In P.N. Juslin & J.A. Sloboda (Eds.), Music and Emotion: Theory and research (pp. 431-449). New York: Oxford University Press.
Kruger, M., Lammers, M. & Ackil, J. (2004) Autobio-graphical memory for musical experiences. In S.D. Lib-
scomb, R. Ashley, R.O. Gjerdingen, & P. Webster (Eds.)
Pillemer, D.B. (2003). Directive functions of autobio-
Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Music
graphical memory: The guiding power of the specific epi-
Perception and Cognition, Northwestern University,
Schulkind, M.D., Hennis, L.K., & Rubin, D.C. (1999) Mu-
Lowis, M.J. (1998) Music and peak experiences. The Man-
sic, emotion, and autobiographical memory: They’re play-
ing your song. Memory and Cognition, 27(6), 948-955.
Pennebaker, J.W., Francis, M.E., & Booth, R.J. (2001).
Sloboda, J.A. & O’Neill, S.A. (2001) Emotions in every-
Linguistic inquiry and word count (LIWC): LIWC 2001.
day listening to music. In P.N. Juslin & J.A. Sloboda
(Eds.), Music and Emotion: Theory and research (pp. 415-429). New York: Oxford University Press.
Pennebaker, J.W., Mehl, M.R., & Niederhoffer, K.G. (2003). Psychological aspects of natural language use: Our
words, our selves. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 547-577.
Approved Bag for RIDOMIL GOLD 1G Fungicide 26612 08-11-28 Page 1 of 7 4 FUNGICIDE RIDOMIL GOLD 1G FUNGICIDE GRANULAR AGRICULTURAL Systemic fungicide for the control of Cavity Spot in Carrots, Damping Off and Stunt in Head Lettuce and Phytophthora Root Rot caused by P hytophthora cactorum in ginseng GUARANTEE: Metalaxyl-M and S-isomer . 1% READ THE LABEL BEFORE
LISTE DER VERBOTENEN PHARMAKOLOGISCH-MEDIZINISCHEN MASSNAHMEN ZUR LEISTUNGSBEEINFLUSSUNG (DOPING-LISTE) gültig ab 1.1.2003 LISTE DES MOYENS PHARMACO-MEDICAUX DEFENDUS EMPLOYES POUR INFLUENCER LES PERFORMANCES (LISTE DES SUBSTANCES DOPANTES INTERDITES) valable dès 1.1.2003 Inhalt / Contenu Verbotene Substanzklassen / Classes de substances interdites II.